"When in Rome, do as the Romans do!"
Nemo is two years old now and she tries to imitate her parents in what she says or does. She doesn't do it perfectly, but with every repetition, she only gets better and faster.
Cultural learning is an inherent part of being human. In fact, in addition to a genetic evolution, modern humans are also a result of cultural evolution. We, as a species, learned by observation - language, tool making, hunting, social skills, rituals; we passively learnt them all by being present in a community or a society and they in turn helped us evolve faster. We could store information outside ourself and we could learn from others without having to wait the changes in our inheritance.
In
one of the least modest statements of intellectual history, Sigmund Freud
considered his work on discovering the human unconscious as one of the three
great revolutions in the history of science. The other two being - the
Copernican revolution and Darwin’s theory of evolution. Despite this apparent
lack of modesty, I do credit Freud for pointing out one salient commonality
underlying these scientific revolutions, which is that they all knock human
arrogance off its pedestal. They all destroy the vision of human uniqueness
that has been cherished for centuries and is corrupting our thoughts even today.
However,
by these standards, a couple of recent papers published in Science have
heralded another scientific revolution – one that takes away the uniqueness of
culture from human existence.
It began
with a simple idea, proposed over half a century ago by the Japanese biologist
Kinji Imanishi, that if animals, like humans can learn from each other, they
will inevitably develop different behaviors in different groups, resulting in
cultural variation within the same species. On the face of it – it is a simple
idea and yet, it has been conclusively proven only now. There has long been a
debate about animal culture that has focused on the mechanisms of behavioral
transmission. Do animals learn from each other the way humans do? If yes, is
this purely imitation or is this based on a goal directed approach? Is animal
learning merely for genetic survival or is there a cultural evolution happening
in the animal world?
Many
studies in the past have looked at animal learning from human sources and have
found little success. But this does not reveal much about animal learning from
other members of the group. It is this specific question that these two papers
have addressed in two different model species – the humpback whales and the Vervet
monkeys.
An
example of the newer lobtail feeding technique wherein the whale pounds on the
surface of the water with his fluke and causes greater clustering of the prey.
B) A group of vervet monkeys being trained to associate taste with corn
kernels.
In
the first report, Allen et al, demonstrate the spread of a new hunting
technique among humpback whales by cultural learning. Their observations are
based on a large-scale study - spanning 27 years and 73,790 sightings of
individually marked humpback whales - in the gulf of Maine where the sand lance
(a popular prey) spawn.
Traditionally,
humpback whales are seen to feed by producing air bubbles. Yes, air bubbles
that drive the tiny prey fish together into large clusters, just under the
surface of the water. The whales usually feed in large groups where one whale
initiates the attack by blowing their large curtains of air bubbles that drive
the prey together and the other whales then join the party with the gaping wide
mouths. This allows the whales to gulp hundreds of water and the fish in them
in a matter of seconds. However, in the year 1980, a whale was seen to do
something different. This rebel whale was seen to whack the surface of the
water with his fluke and the resulting noise further clumped the prey together.
In contrast to bubble feeding, this new tactic was christened as lobtail
feeding.
The
authors then studied the spread and diffusion of this feeding technique across
large populations of humpback whales. Their statistical tests showed that the
spread of this technique was most significantly correlated to the time spent by
the two whales together. Other possible alternatives like genetic spread of the
behavior or individual learning by experience did not stand up to the same
statistical scrutiny.
Spatial distribution of lobtail feeding
A network depicting the social network of
whales sighted at least 20 times. Based on statistical analysis, the spread of
the new feeding technique best correlated with the duration of contact between
the individuals.
Although,
this report is a good indication of cultural learning in animals, it does not
establish the same conclusively.
The
second study by Van de Waal et al, however, cleverly and elegantly overcame the
limitations of this study and established similar principles of learning in
Vervet monkeys.
Using
four groups of wild vervet monkeys, the authors showed that animals from a
group developed their preferences for a particular food source based on the
other members of the group. The animals were first exposed to corn kernels of
two different colors. For two of the groups, one color was palatable while the
other was distasteful. The remaining two groups were trained to find the
exactly opposite colors palatable. As part of this early exposure, all groups
developed a preference for the untreated corn – which was marked by a different
color in two of the groups. This observation is the standard paradigm of animal
learning that we all encounter – where humans and animals learn from their
experience and change future choices.
Interestingly,
the investigators extended their study of color preferences to the next
generation of new infants and to male immigrants from neighboring groups. This
time however, the corn kernels were merely colored with no noxious taste
associated with either color.
Interestingly, they found that 26 of the 27 newborn infants also
developed a preference for the corn color that was preferred by the group. The
behavior of these infants including that of the outlier, established that food
preferences were acquired from the mothers and not through individual
exploration.
Males abandon the food preference learned
in their home group in favor of the local preference of the adopted group.
Column colors indicate the food preference of each male in the original group
and the contrasting food choice in the adopted group. The color of the framing
line indicates the color preference of the adopted group as a whole. The (*)
indicates the color eaten first in the adopted group when not outranked by
local residents.
It
was even more fascinating that the male immigrants originating from groups with
opposite color preferences also adopted the local preferences, without any
exposure to noxious stimuli. This clearly shows that the monkeys are culturally
sensitive to the behavior of the locals and are more likely to adopt them even
though their own experience may suggest the contrary.
Both
these studies ingeniously demonstrate the existence of culture in animal
societies, thus destroying the last uniquely human pedestal.
Slobodkina’sfairy tale of a cap peddler trying to recover his wares from a troop of monkeys was a rather popular tale for many kids. But these studies now suggest that such a piece of
fiction may not be very far from the truth – at least in the case of Vervet
monkeys and whales.
No comments:
Post a Comment